WMM

WMM

TOPIC 2: COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY: 2.1.1 The Working Memory Model (Baddeley and Hitch, 1974).

KEYWORDS

  • SENSORY REGISTER/MEMORY (ICONIC, ECHOIC, HAPTIC, OLFACTORY, GUSTATORY): The Sensory Register is the initial stage of memory that captures sensory information from the environment for a brief period. It includes:

  1. Iconic Memory: Visual sensory memory (images and visual stimuli).

  2. Echoic Memory: Auditory sensory memory (sounds).

  3. Haptic Memory: Tactile sensory memory (touch).

  4. Olfactory Memory: Memory for smells.

  5. Gustatory Memory: Memory for tastes.

  • SHORT-TERM MEMORY (STM): Short-term memory is the temporary storage of information being processed. It typically holds a limited amount of information (around 7 items) for a short duration (approximately 18-30 seconds).

  • LONG-TERM MEMORY (LTM): Long-term memory is the continuous storage of information, lasting from a few minutes to an entire lifetime. It has a much larger capacity than STM and can store different types of information (e.g., declarative, procedural).

  • TRANSFER OF STM TO LTM: The process through which information moves from Short-Term Memory to Long-Term Memory. This typically requires rehearsal and encoding, where data is processed deeply enough to be stored long-term.

  • FORGETTING THROUGH RETRIEVAL FAILURE: A type of forgetting occurs when information is stored in Long-Term Memory but cannot be accessed. Retrieval failure often occurs due to a lack of retrieval cues or context.

  • LINEAR DIRECTION: In memory models, a linear direction refers to the sequential process by which information moves through different memory stores—first to the sensory register, then to STM, and finally to LTM.

  • FORGETTING THROUGH DISPLACEMENT: Forgetting Through Displacement occurs when new information pushes out older information from Short-Term Memory due to its limited capacity.

  • UNITARY STORE: A Unitary Store is a memory model concept suggesting that memory is stored in a single, undifferentiated store rather than separate types (e.g., no division between STM and LTM).

  • MULTIPLE STORES: Unlike a unitary store, Multiple Stores refer to models of memory that suggest distinct types of memory storage (e.g., Sensory Memory, Short-Term Memory, and Long-Term Memory), each with different characteristics.

  • REHEARSAL LOOP: The Rehearsal Loop is the process of repeatedly mentally repeating or verbalising information to keep it in Short-Term Memory or to transfer it to Long-Term Memory.

  • ENCODING: Encoding transforms sensory input into a form that can be stored in memory. It is how information is prepared to be stored in either Short-Term or Long-Term Memory.

  • CAPACITY: Capacity refers to the information held in a particular memory store. For instance, STM has a limited capacity (around seven items), whereas LTM is thought to have a virtually unlimited capacity.

  • DURATION: Duration is when information can be stored in a memory system. For example, STM has a short duration of around 18-30 seconds, whereas LTM can retain information for a much longer, potentially a lifetime.

  • ACOUSTIC ENCODING: Acoustic encoding converts information into sound patterns for memory storage, often used in STM.

  • SEMANTIC ENCODING: Semantic Encoding is encoding information by its meaning, making it easier to recall. This type of encoding is more common in LTM.

  • VISUAL ENCODING: Visual Encoding converts visual information (e.g., images, colours) into a memory trace for storage. This type of encoding is often used in both sensory memory and STM.

  • AMNESIA: Amnesia is a condition characterised by a significant loss of memory, often affecting one’s ability to remember past events (Retrograde) or form new memories (Anterograde).

    Causes:

    • Brain injury or trauma (e.g., concussion).

    • Neurological conditions (e.g., Alzheimer’s disease).

    • Psychological events (e.g., extreme stress or trauma).

  • ANTEROGRADE AMNESIA: Anterograde Amnesia is the inability to form new memories after the onset of an amnesia-causing event. While people can still recall past events, they struggle to retain new information.

    Causes:

    • Damage to the hippocampus or related brain areas (often due to traumatic brain injury, stroke, or certain drugs/alcohol abuse).

    • Diseases like Alzheimer’s that affect the brain's memory systems.

  • RETROGRADE AMNESIA: Retrograde Amnesia is the loss of access to memories formed before the onset of the amnesia-causing event. It can affect recent memories or even older ones, depending on severity.

    Causes:

    • Traumatic brain injury (e.g., concussion, surgery).

    • Psychological trauma or events leading to memory repression.

    • Neurological damage (from conditions like encephalitis or Alzheimer’s disease).

    MEMORY FAILURES AND CAUSES

  • DISPLACEMENT: The process where new information pushes out older information from Short-Term Memory (STM) because of its limited capacity.

    Cause: This happens often when memorising many items at once, leading to forgetting the earlier ones.

  • TRACE DECAY: Memory loss if it is not accessed or rehearsed. Trace Decay is a theory of forgetting in which memory traces (the physical changes in the brain that represent memories) fade and weaken over time when they are not actively rehearsed or use

    Cause: Natural degradation of memory traces in both Short-Term and Long-Term Memory.

  • RETRIEVAL FAILURE: The inability to access a memory stored in Long-Term Memory (LTM), often due to insufficient cues or lack of context.

    Cause: Sometimes known as the "tip-of-the-tongue" phenomenon, it can occur due to insufficient rehearsal or improper encoding, making the memory hard to retrieve when needed

THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE WORKING MEMORY MODEL

As discussed in the summary of Atkinson and Shiffrin’s Multi-Store Model (MSM), the contribution of this foundational theory to memory research cannot be underestimated. Initially, memory was thought to function as a solitary unit in the brain. However, the idea that memory is solely comprised of three stores proved too simplistic to explain the vast complexity of human cognition. One of MSM’s greatest contributions was its ability to inspire the development of more sophisticated models, paving the way for a deeper understanding of memory processes.

One such development was the Working Memory Model (WMM) proposed by Baddeley and Hitch (1974), which redefined the concept of Short-Term Memory (STM). Their research, including the case of KF, challenged the traditional view of STM as a single, unified system. KF, an amnesiac, could transfer visual information from STM to Long-Term Memory (LTM) but struggled to transfer linguistic details. This discrepancy raised a fundamental question: could STM consist of separate systems for visual and linguistic information?

To address this, Baddeley and Hitch proposed that STM was not a single store but comprised multiple specialised components. The original model included three components:

  • The Central Executive: The "boss" of the system, responsible for allocating attention and coordinating the activities of the slave systems. It does not store information but acts as a controller, deciding which tasks require more focus. For example, it might shift attention between processing linguistic information via the Phonological Loop and visual-spatial tasks via the Visuospatial Sketchpad.

  • The Phonological Loop: This subsystem handles verbal and auditory information. It has two parts:

    • The Phonological Store, which temporarily holds sound-based information. For instance, it keeps a phone number you’ve just heard active in your memory for a few seconds.

    • The Articulatory Process rehearses verbal information to prevent it from fading. For example, silently repeating a shopping list to yourself helps you retain it temporarily.

  • The Visuospatial Sketchpad: Often referred to as the “inner eye,” this component processes visual and spatial information. It allows you to visualise and manipulate images in your mind, such as mentally navigating a familiar route or picturing the layout of your living room. The sketchpad has two subdivisions:

    • The Visual Cache stores details like shape and colour.

    • The Inner Scribe rehearses spatial information and tracks movement, such as following the position of a moving car.

By 2001, the model was expanded to include the Episodic Buffer, introduced to address criticisms of the original WMM. This integrative component links information from the Phonological Loop, the Visuospatial Sketchpad, and LTM, creating unified episodes. For example, when reading a story, it combines the visual text (from the Sketchpad), the inner voice narrating the story (from the Loop), and personal knowledge (from LTM) to provide a coherent understanding of the plot.

A critical feature of the WMM is its ability to explain how STM processes linguistic/verbal and visual-spatial information simultaneously. This separation aligns with everyday experiences. For instance, navigating on the phone demonstrates the capacity to handle both modalities concurrently. Similarly, meeting someone for the first time and later recalling their conversation and appearance suggests that STM can manage multiple types of information simultaneously, challenging the MSM’s simpler view of a singular STM.

By distinguishing between these components and illustrating their interactions, Baddeley and Hitch’s WMM provided a dynamic and functional approach to understanding STM, revolutionising memory research. It highlights how the brain processes and integrates different types of information, paving the way for further advancements in understanding human cognition.

RESEARCH THAT SUPPORTS WMM

EVALUATION OF THE WORKING MEMORY MODEL

ADVANTAGES

RESEARCH

Substantial evidence supports the idea of two slave systems within the Working Memory Model, as proposed by Baddeley and Hitch. This evidence comes from various sources, including case studies, experimental studies, and neuroimaging research:

  1. Case Studies: Individuals with brain damage, such as Clive Wearing, have provided insights into the dissociation between different memory systems. Despite severe impairments in episodic memory, individuals like Clive Wearing demonstrate relatively preserved working memory abilities, suggesting the presence of distinct memory systems.

  2. Experimental Studies: Studies utilising dual-task paradigms have consistently shown that individuals struggle to perform two tasks simultaneously if both functions rely on the same cognitive resources. For example, participants may have difficulty simultaneously completing a verbal reasoning task and a verbal memory task, indicating the limited capacity of the phonological loop.

  3. Neuroimaging Research: Neuroimaging techniques such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) have revealed distinct neural networks underlying different components of working memory. For instance, studies have shown that verbal working memory tasks activate regions associated with language processing, while spatial working memory tasks activate regions involved in visuospatial processing. This supports the idea of separate neural substrates for the phonological loop and visuospatial sketchpad.

The Working Memory Model (WMM) is supported by a wealth of research from diverse methodologies, making it one of the most robust and well-validated models in cognitive psychology. Cognitive neuroscience, cognitive neuropsychology, and experimental cognitive psychology contribute to a rich body of evidence that triangulates the model's key claims, enhancing its validity and reliability.

One of the WMM’s greatest strengths is the support from cognitive neuroscience, particularly neuroimaging studies. Techniques like fMRI and PET scans consistently show distinct brain regions activated during verbal and visual-spatial tasks, such as the left prefrontal cortex for linguistic tasks and the right prefrontal cortex for spatial processing. These findings align with the WMM’s distinction between the Phonological Loop and the Visuospatial Sketchpad, providing strong neural evidence for the model. Neuroimaging methods are highly objective and robust, reducing subjectivity or experimental bias concerns.

Additionally, cognitive neuropsychology, including case studies like KF (Shallice & Warrington), offers valuable insights by illustrating how damage to specific brain areas can impair certain aspects of working memory while leaving others intact. KF’s difficulties with verbal STM but intact visual-spatial STM strongly support the WMM’s assertion that STM is not a unitary store. However, neuropsychological evidence can suffer from validity issues due to the reliance on single cases that may not be generalised to the broader population.

Experimental cognitive psychology, such as Baddeley and Hitch’s dual-task experiments, further bolsters the WMM. These studies demonstrate that individuals can perform verbal and visual tasks simultaneously without interference, consistent with separate slave systems. Yet, the artificial nature of laboratory tasks raises concerns about ecological validity, as the controlled environments may not accurately reflect real-life memory use.

Despite these limitations, the convergence of findings across all three methodologies—neuroscience, neuropsychology, and experimental psychology—creates a robust case for the WMM. This triangulation of evidence enhances confidence in the model, as the strengths of another often mitigate the weaknesses of one method. For instance, while cognitive neuropsychology may lack generalisability, its findings are supported by robust and objective neuroimaging results, lending greater overall validity to the model.

SYNOPSIS OF OF WMM RESEARCH:

The Working Memory Model (WMM) has been evaluated through various methods, including case studies, experimental studies, and brain scans. Each method has inherent flaws: case studies cannot be readily generalized to the broader population, experimental studies may lack mundane realism and ecological validity, and brain scans may not capture the full complexity of cognitive processes. However, when considered collectively, along with brain scan data, the evidence overwhelmingly supports the notion that working memory is not a singular unit.

SUPPORT FROM NEUROSCIENCE

The WMM is supported by neuroimaging research that demonstrates distinct brain regions linked to its components:

  • The phonological loop is associated with Broca’s area and Wernicke’s area in the left hemisphere, activated during verbal tasks.

  • The visuospatial sketchpad involves the occipital and parietal lobes responsible for processing spatial and visual information.

  • The central executive is linked to the prefrontal cortex, particularly the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, active during tasks requiring attentional control.

  • While newer to the model, the episodic buffer likely involves the hippocampus and other areas involved in multimodal integration.

This evidence, derived from robust methodologies like fMRI and PET scans, strengthens the WMM’s validity and demonstrates how its components operate in the brain.

STM NO MORE

The Working Memory Model (WMM) provides a more dynamic explanation of short-term memory than earlier models like the Multi-Store Model (MSM). It emphasises active manipulation of information, highlighting how memory processes involve reorganising, prioritising, and integrating data. Unlike the passive depiction of STM in the MSM, the WMM portrays memory as the seat of consciousness, where real-time tasks like problem-solving, multitasking, and learning occur.

For example, dual-task experiments show individuals can manage two simultaneous tasks using distinct working memory systems (e.g., verbal and visual). This demonstrates the model’s versatility and ability to explain real-world cognitive function diversity.

APPLICATIONS TO THE REAL WORLD

The WMM has significant real-world applications, particularly in understanding and addressing cognitive challenges and educational needs.

  • Managing Mental Health Conditions: The WMM aids in understanding conditions like ADHD, OCD, Tourette’s syndrome, and schizophrenia, where central executive dysfunction is evident. For example, interventions targeting attentional control and impulse regulation (core functions of the central executive) can improve symptoms.

  • Diagnosing Educational Difficulties: The model explains learning disorders such as dyslexia and dyspraxia, where phonological loop deficits affect reading and verbal processing. Tailored interventions, such as breaking tasks into smaller chunks, can reduce cognitive load and improve outcomes.

  • Detecting Neurodegenerative Diseases: Declines in working memory, especially in the episodic buffer and central executive, are early indicators of dementia. Assessing working memory capacity allows for early diagnosis and intervention.

  • Improving Educational Practices: Teachers can use strategies like chunking, mnemonics, and multimodal teaching to enhance students’ working memory and learning efficiency.

CRITICISMS WORKING MEMORY

LACK OF NEUROBIOLOGICAL SPECIFICITY

The Working Memory Model (WMM) is supported by neuroimaging studies, identifying brain regions linked to its components. However, the model fails to explain how these brain areas interact to perform complex memory tasks. Memory processes involve interconnected networks, not isolated brain regions. For example:

  • Prefrontal Cortex: Governs attention and decision-making, functions attributed to the central executive.

  • Anterior Cingulate Cortex: Resolves conflicts, such as prioritising competing tasks when the phonological loop and visuospatial sketchpad are overloaded.

  • Parietal Lobes: Coordinate spatial awareness and visual-spatial information.

These regions work as part of a distributed system. For instance, navigating to a new location while on the phone requires cooperation between the visuospatial sketchpad, phonological loop, and central executive. The WMM, however, simplifies these interactions, failing to explain how components communicate to perform real-world tasks.

Additionally, advances in neuroscience reveal that brain regions share responsibilities across tasks. For example, the anterior cingulate and parietal lobes are active in verbal and spatial tasks, suggesting that the boundaries between WMM components are less clear-cut than the model implies.

Summary: While the WMM identifies key brain regions involved in working memory, it oversimplifies their interactions. Incorporating these dynamic relationships would align the model more closely with modern neuroscience.

ROLE OF THE CENTRAL EXECUTIVE

The central executive (CE), described as the "boss" of working memory, is one of the least understood aspects of the WMM. It is said to allocate attention and manage the phonological loop and visuospatial sketchpad. However, the WMM lacks clarity on how these functions are performed, earning the CE the nickname “black box” due to its vague conceptualisation.

Neuroimaging Evidence:

  • Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex: Engages in holding and manipulating information.

  • Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex: Involved in decision-making and integrating emotions.

  • Anterior Cingulate Cortex: Monitors conflicts and errors when tasks compete for resources.

This evidence suggests that executive functions rely on a distributed network of brain regions, challenging the WMM’s depiction of the CE as a singular component. For example, solving a math problem while listening to instructions requires the CE to coordinate multiple brain areas, prioritise tasks, and resolve conflicts.

Case Study Evidence:

  • Patient EVR (Eslinger et al.): After surgery to remove a tumour, EVR could reason well but struggled with decision-making, demonstrating that executive functions are modular and distributed rather than unified.

Criticism: The WMM’s portrayal of the CE oversimplifies its role, ignoring the complexity of overlapping brain systems involved in attention, multitasking, and decision-making. Alternative theories, such as the Global Workspace Theory, provide a more detailed account of attentional control, highlighting this limitation.

INCOMPLETE EXPLANATION OF LONG-TERM MEMORY INTERACTIONS

The episodic buffer, introduced to explain how working memory interacts with long-term memory (LTM), offers only a vague account of these processes. For example:

  • Retrieval from LTM: The WMM does not explain how working memory retrieves schemas, facts, or meanings from LTM to complete tasks like interpreting a story or solving a problem.

  • Storage into LTM: Similarly, the transition from working memory to LTM—through processes like rehearsal or organisation—is not explicitly tied to the episodic buffer or other components.

Examples:

  • Navigating using the visuospatial sketchpad relies on maps stored in LTM.

  • Conversations involve the phonological loop drawing on LTM for word meanings and prior context.

  • Problem-solving requires the central executive to retrieve and apply schemas from LTM.

The WMM does not sufficiently explain how these bidirectional relationships between working memory and LTM are managed.

Alternative Models:

  • Logie’s Hierarchical Model: This suggests that working memory relies on activated LTM representations rather than modular systems.

  • Cowen’s Embedded Process Model argues that working memory is simply an active subset of LTM, further blurring distinctions.

Summary: The WMM needs refinement to explain how working memory and LTM interact during encoding, retrieval, and manipulation processes.

LIEBERMAN’S CRITIQUE OF THE VISUOSPATIAL SKETCHPAD

Lieberman argues that the WMM conflates visual and spatial processing in conceptualising the visuospatial sketchpad (VSS), overlooking how these processes can operate independently.

Evidence:

  • Blind individuals demonstrate strong spatial reasoning through auditory or tactile cues, forming mental maps without visual input.

  • Tasks such as estimating distance or navigating environments rely on spatial reasoning that does not require visual imagery.

This suggests that spatial processing is a distinct cognitive function separate from visual processing. The WMM fails to account for these distinctions, oversimplifying the VSS.

Implications:

  • The WMM could be refined by separating visual and spatial components, reflecting their independent roles in cognition.

EMOTIONAL AND CULTURAL INFLUENCES

The WMM largely ignores how emotions and cultural contexts influence working memory processes:

  1. Stress and Anxiety:

    • High stress reduces prefrontal cortex activity, impairing the CE’s ability to allocate attention or switch tasks.

    • Anxiety-related rumination consumes attentional resources, limiting the capacity of the phonological loop and visuospatial sketchpad.

  2. Cultural Variations:

    • Individuals from logographic cultures (e.g., Chinese) exhibit stronger visuospatial processing due to reliance on visual memory for reading.

    • Alphabetic cultures (e.g., English) depend more on the phonological loop for decoding letters and sounds.

The WMM risks oversimplification and reduced applicability across diverse populations by neglecting these factors.

SUMMARY OF LIMITATIONS

  • Oversimplification of Neural Mechanisms: Fails to account for the distributed networks supporting working memory tasks.

  • Vague Central Executive: Lacks specificity regarding allocating attention and resolving conflicts.

  • Limited Integration with LTM: Provides insufficient detail on how working memory and LTM interact.

  • Cultural and Sensory Oversights: Ignores variations in memory processes across populations.

CONCLUSION

The WMM remains an influential framework for understanding short-term memory processes, but its limitations underscore the need for refinement. Incorporating findings from neuroscience and addressing cultural and emotional influences could enhance its explanatory power, making it a more comprehensive model of working memory

HOW TO WRITE A03 FOR ESSAYS

Critical Evaluation of the Working Memory Model

Strengths

A key consensus in contemporary cognitive psychology is that short-term memory (STM) is not a singular, unitary entity, as once proposed by the multistore model, but is instead comprised of multiple components or subsystems. This shift was largely driven by the working memory model, which supersedes the simplistic, one-part view of STM. By suggesting that short-term memory includes distinct subsystems, such as the phonological loop and the visuospatial sketchpad, the working memory model provides a more nuanced and robust framework for understanding how temporary information is processed.

The model also offers a much broader and more comprehensive explanation of cognitive functions compared to the multistore model. It accounts for a diverse range of tasks, including verbal reasoning, comprehension, reading, problem-solving, and visual-spatial processing. Its utility across real-life tasks is evident, as it provides explanations for the cognitive processes involved in activities like reading (phonological loop), problem-solving (central executive), and navigation (visuospatial sketchpad).

An influential piece of evidence supporting the working memory model comes from the KF case study. KF suffered brain damage due to a motorcycle accident, which specifically impaired his short-term memory for verbal information, leaving his ability to process visual information largely unaffected. This clinical observation supports the notion that distinct subsystems exist within short-term memory, with the phonological loop responsible for verbal information and the visuospatial sketchpad handling visual-spatial information.

The working memory model further differentiates itself from the multistore model by deemphasizing the role of rehearsal in maintaining information in STM. While the multistore model prioritizes rehearsal as a primary mechanism for information retention, the working memory model provides a more flexible understanding of how memory is structured and utilized without overemphasizing rehearsal.

Empirical Support for Working Memory

The validity of the working memory model has been supported by extensive empirical evidence, particularly from dual-task experiments, such as the study by Baddeley and Hitch (1976). These studies demonstrate that different components of working memory can operate simultaneously without interference if they utilize distinct subsystems. The following two predictions of the working memory model are well-supported by such findings:

  1. Interference between tasks: When two tasks engage the same working memory component, performance on both tasks is impaired.

  2. Simultaneous performance of unrelated tasks: When two tasks engage different components, they can be performed together as efficiently as if done separately.

In their seminal 1976 study, Baddeley and Hitch tested these predictions by asking participants to perform a dual-task experiment. One task required participants to repeat a list of digits (phonological loop), while another involved answering true/false questions related to verbal reasoning (central executive). The results revealed that as the number of digits increased, participants took longer to answer the reasoning questions, but they did not make more errors. This finding suggests that the phonological loop and central executive were functioning independently, supporting the working memory model.

Brain Imaging Studies

Neuroimaging studies have also provided insight into the neural basis of the working memory model. Research using techniques like fMRI has identified distinct neural activations corresponding to different subsystems of working memory. For instance, tasks involving phonological storage often activate regions in the left hemisphere associated with language processing, while visuospatial tasks tend to activate right hemisphere areas, particularly the parietal cortex.

However, the results from neuroimaging studies remain inconclusive, with inconsistent localization of brain activity linked to verbal and visuospatial working memory. Meta-analyses have often failed to confirm a consistent pattern of activation, suggesting that the neural underpinnings of working memory are more complex than initially assumed. There is also considerable overlap in activation patterns, which may indicate the involvement of the episodic buffer and shared executive resources.

As Baddeley (2012) notes, working memory likely relies on a distributed and interactive network of brain regions rather than being confined to specific, localized areas. The interrelationship of working memory with other cognitive systems, such as attention and long-term memory, complicates efforts to pinpoint clear anatomical localization. Despite this, neuroimaging studies continue to provide valuable insights into the neural mechanisms underlying working memory.

Weaknesses

Despite its strengths, the working memory model is not without its criticisms. One significant critique comes from Lieberman (1980), who challenges the assumption that the visuospatial sketchpad (VSS) deals with both spatial and visual information. Lieberman points out that blind individuals, who have no visual experiences, often exhibit excellent spatial awareness. He argues that the VSS should be divided into two separate components: one for visual information and one for spatial processing.

Another limitation of the working memory model is the vague and poorly defined role of the central executive. While it is described as the system that controls attention and coordinates the other components of working memory, its specific function remains unclear. The capacity of the central executive has never been directly measured, and the exact processes it engages in are still debated.

Additionally, the working memory model focuses exclusively on short-term memory, neglecting the broader context of memory systems, such as sensory memory (SM) and long-term memory (LTM). This narrow focus limits the model’s comprehensiveness as an explanation of memory as a whole. Moreover, the model does not account for changes in cognitive processing over time, particularly those that occur with practice or increased experience. These changes in processing speed and efficiency are a well-documented phenomenon, yet the working memory model does not provide an explanation for them.

State-Based Models of Working Memory

While Baddeley and Hitch's multi-component model has been influential, newer theories of working memory have emerged, particularly state-based models. These models suggest that working memory does not rely on dedicated storage systems like the phonological loop or visuospatial sketchpad. Instead, they propose that working memory arises from the temporary activation of representations that already exist in long-term memory or sensory/perceptual systems.

For example, when trying to recall a phone number, you are activating your long-term memory of number concepts. Similarly, remembering the location of your keys involves activating your mental map of the room. In state-based models, working memory relies on attention to these internal representations, temporarily enhancing their activity.

Recent research using multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA) of fMRI data has provided evidence supporting state-based models of working memory. Studies by Lewis-Peacock and Postle (2008) demonstrated that patterns of neural activity related to long-term memory content could predict the information being held in working memory. Additionally, research on sensory cortices has shown that perceptual information is retained in working memory through stimulus-specific activity patterns, supporting the view that working memory involves activation of long-term memory and perceptual systems rather than dedicated buffers.

In conclusion, while the working memory model remains a foundational framework in cognitive psychology, it is not without its limitations. Newer state-based models provide alternative explanations that challenge the assumptions of dedicated storage systems, offering a more flexible understanding of how working memory operates. Further research into both neuroimaging and cognitive processing will continue to refine our understanding of these complex system

INTERESTING VIDEOS ON WMM

Rebecca Sylvia

I am a Londoner with over 30 years of experience teaching psychology at A-Level, IB, and undergraduate levels. Throughout my career, I’ve taught in more than 40 establishments across the UK and internationally, including Spain, Lithuania, and Cyprus. My teaching has been consistently recognised for its high success rates, and I’ve also worked as a consultant in education, supporting institutions in delivering exceptional psychology programmes.

I’ve written various psychology materials and articles, focusing on making complex concepts accessible to students and educators. In addition to teaching, I’ve published peer-reviewed research in the field of eating disorders.

My career began after earning a degree in Psychology and a master’s in Cognitive Neuroscience. Over the years, I’ve combined my academic foundation with hands-on teaching and leadership roles, including serving as Head of Social Sciences.

Outside of my professional life, I have two children and enjoy a variety of interests, including skiing, hiking, playing backgammon, and podcasting. These pursuits keep me curious, active, and grounded—qualities I bring into my teaching and consultancy work. My personal and professional goals include inspiring curiosity about human behaviour, supporting educators, and helping students achieve their full potential.

https://psychstory.co.uk
Previous
Previous

THE COGNITIVE APPROACH

Next
Next

MSM FEATURES OF MEMORY